Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem

dc.contributor.advisorDurán, Martha Consuelo
dc.contributor.authorMartínez Avila, Juan Carlos
dc.date.accessioned2012-11-23T15:18:29Z
dc.date.available2012-11-23T15:18:29Z
dc.date.created2002
dc.date.issued2012-11-23
dc.identifier.citationAnzeloni, V.Z., Motta, V., Lima, G., & Brandao, M. L. (1995). Behavioral and pharmacological validation of the elevated plus maze constructed with transparente walls. Brazilian Journal Medical Biology Research, 28, 597- 601
dc.identifier.citationBarnett, S. A. (1958) Exploratory Behavior. British Journal of Psychology, 21, 15-45
dc.identifier.citationBerlyne, D.E. (1967) Curiosity and exploration. Science, 153, 25-33.
dc.identifier.citationBlanchard, D.C. & Blanchard, R.J. (1988) Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. Annual Review Psychology, 39, 43-68.
dc.identifier.citationBlanchard, Robert, J., & Blanchard, Caroline, D. (1990) An ethoexperimental analysis of defense, fear, and anxiety. En McNaughton N. & Andrews G. (Eds.), Anxiety, (pp. 124-133). New Zealand: University of Otago press.
dc.identifier.citationBolles, R.C. (1972) Reinforcement, expectancy and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394 - 409.
dc.identifier.citationBurkhard, B. & Domjan, M. (1993) Principios de aprendizaje y de conducta, Madrid: Debate.
dc.identifier.citationChance, P. (1995) Aprendizaje y conducta, México: Manual Moderno.
dc.identifier.citationCruz, P., Frei, F., & Graeff, F. (1994) Ethopharmacological análisis of rat behavior on the elevated plus-maze. Biochemestry and Behavior, 49, 171- 176
dc.identifier.citationCommins, D. (1932). A note on the learning of elevated mazes by rats. Journal of Gen. Psychol., 41: 481-482.
dc.identifier.citationDarwin, Ch. (reimpreso 1985). The expression of the emotion in man and animals. New York: Philosophical Library.
dc.identifier.citationAmerican Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statiscal Manual of Mental Disorders (4a. Ed.). Washington D.C
dc.identifier.citationFernandes, C. & Files, S. (1996) The influence of open arm ledges and maze experience in the elevated plus - maze. Pharmacology, Biochemestry and Behavior, 54, 1, 31-40.
dc.identifier.citationFile, S. and Zangrossi Jr. (1993) “One - Trial tolerance” to the anxiolytic actions of benzodiazepines in the elevated plus-maze, or the development of a phobic state?. Psychopharmacology, 110, 240-244.
dc.identifier.citationFile, S., Zangrossi, H., Viana, M., and Graeff, G. (1993) Trial 2 in the elevated plus-maze: a different form of fear?. Psychopharmacology, 111, 491-494
dc.identifier.citationGraeff, F. G. (1994) Neuroanatomy and neurotransmitter regulation of defensive behaviors and related emotions in mammals. Brazilian Journal Medical Biology Research, 27, 811-829.
dc.identifier.citationGray, J. A. (1982) Précis of the neuropsychology of anxiety: An enquiry into the functions of the septo-hipocampal system. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 5, 469-534
dc.identifier.citationGriebel, G., Blanchard, C., Jung, A., Masuda, C., & Blanchard, R. (1995). 5- HT1A agonists modulate mouse antipredator defensive differently from the 5-HT2A antagonist pirenperone. Pharmacology Biochemestry and Behavior, 51, 235-244.
dc.identifier.citationHalliday, M.S. (1967). Exploratory behaviour in elevated and enclosed mazes. Q. J. Experimental Psychology, 19, 254-263
dc.identifier.citationHandley, S., & Mithani, S. (1984). Effects of alpha-adrenoceptor agonists and antagonists in a maze-exploration model of ‘fear’- motivated behaviour. Archives of Pharmacology, 327, 1-5
dc.identifier.citationHull, C.L. (1952) A behavior system. New Haven, Conn.: Yale University
dc.identifier.citationKlein, S. (1994) Aprendizaje. (2a edición) Madrid: McGraw Hill.
dc.identifier.citationKrebs, J.R. & Davies, N. B. (1996). Seleção natural, ecología e comportamento. En Krebs, J.R. & Davies, N.B. (Eds.), Introdução à ecologia comportamental (3a. Edição), (pp. 4-23). Brasil: Atheneu.
dc.identifier.citationLedoux, E. J. (1986). Sensory systems and emotion: A model of affective processing. Integr Psychiatry, 4, 237-248.
dc.identifier.citationLester, D. (1967). Effects of fear upon exploratory behaviour. Psychon. Science, 9, 117-118.
dc.identifier.citationLister, R. (1990). Ethologically-based animal models of anxiety disorders. Pharmacology therapy, 46, 321-340.
dc.identifier.citationLore, R., Kam, B. & Newby, V. (1967). Visual and nonvisual depth avoidance in young and adult rats. Journal of comparative and physiological psychology, 64, 3, 525-528
dc.identifier.citationMcReynolds, P. (1971). Behavioral choice as a function novelty -seeking and anxiety-avoidance motivations. Psychological reports, 29, 3-6.
dc.identifier.citationMillenson, J.R. (1967). Principios de análisis conductual México: Trillas
dc.identifier.citationMiller, N.E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 38, pp. 89-101.
dc.identifier.citationMiles, W.R. (1930) The comparative learning of rats on elevated an alleymazes of the same pattern. J. Comparative Psychology, 10: 237 – 261.
dc.identifier.citationMontgomery, K. (1955). The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior. Journal Comparative Physiology Psychology, 48, 254-260.
dc.identifier.citationMontgomery, K. & Monkman, J. (1955). The relation between fear and exploratory behavior. Journal Comparative Physiology Psychology, 48, 132- 136.
dc.identifier.citationMorato, S., & Catrechini, P. (1989). Effects of floor surface and environmental illumination on exploratory activity in the elevated plus - maze. Brazilian Journal Medical Biology Research, 22, 707-710
dc.identifier.citationOlton, D.S. (1979). Mazes, maps and memory. American Psychologist, 34, 583 – 596.
dc.identifier.citation. Pávlov, I. (1968). Fisiología y Psicología. Madrid: Alianza
dc.identifier.citationPellow, S., Chopin, P., File, S. & Briley, M. (1985). Validation of open: Closed arm entries in an elevated plus - maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J. Neuroscience Methods, 14, 149-167.
dc.identifier.citationRodger, R.J., Cao, B.J., Dalvi, A. & Holmes, A. (1997). Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research, 30, 289-304.
dc.identifier.citationRussel, P. (1973). Relationships between exploratory behaviour and fear: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 64, 417-433.
dc.identifier.citationSchiffman, H., Lore, R., Passafiume, J. & Robert, N. (1970). Role of vibrissae for depth perception in the rat (rattus novergicus). Animal Behavior, 18, 290-292.
dc.identifier.citationSheldon, A. (1969). Preference for familiar versus novel stimuli as a function of the familiarity of the environment. Journal Comparative Physiology Psychology, 67, 516-521.
dc.identifier.citationSkinner, B.F. (1975). La conducta de los organismos: Un análisis experimental. Barcelona: Fontanella.
dc.identifier.citationTierney, Ann, J. (1986) The evolution of learned and innate behavior: Contributions from genetic and neurobiology to a theory of behavioral evolution. Animal learning and behavior, 14 (4), 339-348
dc.identifier.citationTreit, D. (1991) Animal models for the study of anti-anxiety agents: A review. Neuroscience & Behavioral Reviews, 9, 203-222.
dc.identifier.citationTreit, D., Menard, J., Royan, C. (1993) Anxiogenic stimuli in the elevated plus-maze. Psychopharmacology, 44, 463-469.
dc.identifier.citationVan Der Kloot, W.G. (1971). Aprendizaje. En Van Der Kloot, W.G. ( autor), Comportamiento, (pp.121-149). México: Continental.
dc.identifier.citationWalk, R. D. & Gibson, E. (1957) Behavior of light and dark-reared rats on a visual cliff. Science, 126, 80-81.
dc.identifier.citationWeiss, Susan, R.B. & Uhde, Thomas, W. (1990). Animal models of anxiety. En Ballenger, C. James. (Eds.), Serie: Frontiers of Clinical Neuroscience, 8, (pp. 3-27). New York: Liss
dc.identifier.citationWilliams, D. (1971) Maze exploration in the rat under different levels of illumination. Animal Behavior, 19, 365-367.
dc.identifier.citationKupfermann, I. (1991). Learning and Memory. En Kandel, E.R., Schwartz, J.H. & Jessell, T.M. (Eds.), Principles of neural science (Third edition), (pp. 997-1008). New York: Elsevier.
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/10818/4099
dc.description87 Páginas.
dc.description.abstractEl objetivo del presente estudio fue establecer los efectos de la altura del borde y la luminosidad sobre los comportamientos de evitación en los brazos abiertos del Laberinto en Cruz Elevado (LCE). La medida de la dependiente fue hecha a través de la frecuencia de entradas a los brazos abiertos, tiempo de permanencia y frecuencia de aparición del comportamiento de "estiramiento" en el cuadrado central. Los resultados indican que la fuente ansiogénica del LCE se encuentra determinada no solamente por actividad tigmotáxica. Así, la aversión a los brazos abiertos no se puede atribuir únicamente a este tipo de estimulación, y por el contrario existe la posibilidad de combinaciones y establecimiento de jerarquías en la información aversiva presente en esta zona del LCE.es_CO
dc.language.isoeses_CO
dc.publisherUniversidad de la Sabana
dc.sourceUniversidad de la Sabana
dc.sourceIntellectum Repositorio Universidad de la Sabana
dc.subjectPsicología-Investigaciónes_CO
dc.subjectPsicología comparadaes_CO
dc.subjectEtologíaes_CO
dc.subjectAnimales de laboratorioes_CO
dc.subjectPsicología experimentales_CO
dc.titleEfectos de las variables luminosidad y altura de bordes en los brazos abiertos del Laberinto en Cruz Elevado (L.C.E.) sobre las conductas operantes de evitación en ratas albinas wistares_CO
dc.typebachelorThesis
dc.publisher.programPsicología
dc.publisher.departmentFacultad de Psicología
dc.identifier.local85622
dc.identifier.localTE04413
dc.type.localTesis de pregrado
dc.type.hasVersionpublishedVersion
dc.rights.accessRightsopenAccess
dc.creator.degreePsicólogo


Ficheros en el ítem

Thumbnail

Este ítem aparece en la(s) siguiente(s) colección(ones)

Mostrar el registro sencillo del ítem